science
Norman Allan
alternative
medicine
email
  science and philosophy      history/misc     fiction       gallery      biography         blog

 

The animal mind can be quite complex. Consider the work of Dr. Pepperberg with her African grey parrot, Alex. Dr. Pepperberg writes: "... During the 1940's and 1950's, European researchers such as Koehler and Logler had shown that Grey parrots could learn symbolic and conceptual tasks often considered as pre- or co-requisites for complex cognitive and communicative skills."

Pepperberg has worked for many years with an African grey that she calls Alex.  "Alex has mastered tasks once thought to be beyond the capacity of all but humans or certain nonhuman primates. Not only can he produce and comprehend English labels, but he also understands concepts of category, "same/different", absence, quantity, and size (my highlighting).
   "Alex has learned over 40 object labels: paper, key, nut, wood, ...
   "He has functional use of "no", phrases such as "come here", "I want X", and "Wanna go Y" where X and Y are appropriate labels for objects or locations...
   "He has also acquired attribute labels. He can identify 7 colors, rose (red), blue, green, yellow, orange, grey, and purple. He labels 5 different shapes as 2-, 3-, 4-, 5-, or 6-cornered objects. He uses "two", "three", "four", "five", and "sih" (six) to distinguish quantities of objects, including groups of novel items, heterogeneous collections, and sets in which objects are randomly arrayed. He combines attribute and object labels to identify proficiently, request, refuse, categorize, and quantify over 100 different objects, including those that vary from training exemplars. His accuracy averages approximately 80% on tests of these abilities.
   "Alex comprehends a limited concept of "category". He has learned not only that "green", for example, is one instance of the category "color", but also that, for a particularly colored and shaped object, "green" and "3-corner" represent two different categories of its markable attributes. Thus he categorizes such objects with respect to either attribute based on our vocal query of "What color?" or "What shape?" Because the same exemplar can be the subject of shape and color queries at different times, Alex must be able to change his basis for classification. Such ability to "reclassify" is thought to indicate the presence of "abstract aptitude".
   "Alex has also learned abstract concepts of "same", "different", and to respond vocally to the presence or absence of information about these concepts. Thus when shown two identical objects or ones that vary with respect to some or all of the attributes of color, shape, and material, Alex responds with the appropriate category label as to which attribute is "same" or "different" for any combination; if, however, nothing is same or different, he replies "none". He responds accurately to objects, colors, shapes, and materials not used in training, including those for which he has no labels. Furthermore, he is indeed responding to the specific questions, and not merely on the basis of his training and the physical attributes of the objects: His accuracy was above chance levels on questions such as "What's same?" for a green wooden triangle and a blue wooden triangle. If he were ignoring the question and responding on the basis of his prior training, he would have determined, and responded with the label for the one anomalous attribute (in this case, "color"). Instead, he responded appropriately [i.e., "shape", though "mah-mah" (matter) would also have been correct].
 
    "We have further data supporting his comprehension skills. Alex was able to view different collections of 7 exemplars (each collection chosen from among 100 objects of various combinations of shapes, colors, and materials), be asked 1 of 4 possible vocal questions, each of which requested a different type of information (e.g., "What color is object-X?") about a single object in the collection, and reply vocally to each question. A correct response indicated that he understood all the parts of the question and used these parts to guide the search for the one object in the collection that provided the requested information. Alex responded with an accuracy of 81.3%. We also have evidence for comprehension of his own use of labels: If a trainer incorrectly responds to Alex's requests (e.g., substitutes an unrequested item), he generally responds (approx. 75% of the time) by saying "No" and repeating the initial request. Finally, a soon-to-be published manuscript demonstrates that he comprehends a relative concept: He responds correctly to questions - asking which of two objects is the bigger or smaller, and responds "none" if they are of equal size."

One of the striking things, watching Alex on television, is how fast he is with his answers. When asked a question like "how many red triangles", he can see and answer quicker than I (mind you, he's had practice). Another neat thing, when he would make a mistake (perhaps intentionally) he would say, "Sorry".